Skip to main content
/v1/supplement/title

Effect of intermittent isometric handgrip exercise protocol with short exercise duration on cognitive performance

Abstract

The handgrip exercise, a small muscle exercise, is useful for exercise therapy, particularly in the elderly and bedridden patients. The isometric handgrip (IHG) exercise has been utilized in training programs to reduce resting blood pressure; however, the acute effects of the IHG exercise on cognitive performance are not fully understood. The present study aimed to investigate the effect of an intermittent IHG exercise protocol with short exercise duration, which minimizes the arterial blood pressure response to exercise, on cognitive performance. Twenty-two young healthy subjects performed the intermittent IHG exercise protocol, which consisted of 30-s IHG and 45-s recovery × 16 trials; the exercise intensity of the IHG exercise was 30% of the maximal voluntary contraction. Cognitive performance was evaluated before and after the exercise with the Go/No-Go and memory recognition tasks. Specifically, the reaction time (RT) and performance accuracy were measured. The intermittent IHG exercise protocol did not change the RT or performance accuracy of either the Go/No-Go task (P = 0.222 and P = 0.260, respectively) or the memory recognition task (P = 0.427 and P = 0.245, respectively). These findings suggest that the intermittent IHG exercise protocol with short exercise duration may not provide enough stimulation to improve cognitive performance despite being useful as a safe exercise therapy in the elderly and in patients with cardiovascular disease.

Background

Cognitive function is an essential component of most daily activities [1, 2], but it is changeable and determined by many physiological factors. For example, aging and cardiovascular disease have been reported to attenuate cognitive function [3, 4]. Therefore, by one method or another, cognitive dysfunction must be prevented to maintain quality of life for the elderly and any other patients [5]. Previous studies have reported that both dynamic and static (isometric) exercise training improved cognitive performance and cardiovascular function [6,7,8,9]. However, in general, dynamic exercise rather than isometric exercise is recommended as exercise therapy in the elderly or any patients who have a higher blood pressure response to exercise [10,11,12,13,14,15] because the dynamic exercise-induced increase in arterial blood pressure (ABP) is lower than that induced by isometric exercises [16].

Importantly, it is difficult in some cases for the elderly and patients who have limitations moving large muscle groups to perform dynamic whole-body exercises, e.g., cycling, walking, etc. On the other hand, it has been reported that exercise training using the isometric handgrip (IHG) exercise protocol (2-min IHG at 30% maximal voluntary contraction (MVC) and 3-min recovery × 4 trials) reduces resting blood pressure and improves arterial stiffness as well as endothelial function [17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24]. Moreover, our recent study demonstrated that the acute IHG exercise protocol improved cognitive performance [25]. These findings indicate that the IHG exercise may be a better exercise mode for cardiovascular and cognitive therapies, particularly in the elderly and bedridden patients, compared with dynamic whole-body exercise because the IHG exercise is a useful small muscle exercise that can be easily performed in various conditions, such as in a supine position in a hospital bed.

One important problem using an isometric small muscle exercise for the elderly and patients with cardiovascular disease is that their exercise pressor response is larger compared with healthy young adults [10, 26, 27]. Exercise with a large blood pressure response is often contraindicated in patients with cardiovascular disease because an unusually large increase in blood pressure during exercise may increase the risk of cardiovascular events. In addition, the IHG exercise may augment the blood pressure response more than dynamic whole-body exercise [16].

It is noteworthy that a previous study [28] indicated that in the IHG exercise protocol, the ABP response can be modified by maintaining the total exercise volume while decreasing the contraction time and increasing the frequency of IHG contractions. Indeed, our pilot study [29] confirmed that the intermittent IHG exercise protocol with short duration maintaining the same total exercise volume as the traditional IHG exercise protocol resulted in a 26% lower pressor response to exercise (mean ABP; 99 mmHg vs. 124 mmHg, P = 0.007, n = 7) compared with that of the IHG exercise protocol that reduces resting ABP as reported previously [19, 20]. More recently, our study [25] demonstrated that the acute traditional IHG exercise protocol improved cognitive performance. Moreover, previous studies [30,31,32] using dynamic exercise with whole body or large muscle groups suggest that the same total exercise volume would have an equally positive effect on cognitive performance regardless of different exercise duration and frequency. Therefore, we hypothesized that the acute intermittent IHG exercise protocol with short exercise duration that maintains the total exercise volume as the traditional IHG protocol [25] improves cognitive performance with a lower blood pressure response.

In the present study, we investigated the acute effect of the intermittent IHG exercise protocol with short exercise duration (30-s IHG at 30% MVC and 45-s recovery × 16 trials) on cognitive performance. The findings of the present study are expected to provide important information as to whether the intermittent IHG exercise with short exercise duration is a useful exercise mode for preventing cognitive dysfunction, particularly in the elderly and any patients who have a high exercise pressor reflex and limitations with moving large muscles.

Methods

Ethical approval

This experimental protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Toyo University (Approval Number: TU2019-039), and each subject provided written informed consent prior to participation in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Subjects

Twenty-two healthy volunteers participated in this study (18 men and 4 women; mean age, 22.0 ± 1.0 years; height, 169.6 ± 7.9 cm; weight, 60.9 ± 10.4 kg). None of the subjects had cerebrovascular or cardiovascular disease; all subjects were not taking any medications and were non-smokers. Each subject abstained from caffeine for 12 h and strenuous exercise and alcohol for 12 h. The experiment was performed at least 4 h after a light meal.

Experimental procedure

On the same day, before the experiment, all subjects practiced the intermittent IHG exercise protocol and completed at least one practice block of each task (Go/No-Go tasks; 30 trials and memory recognition; 10 words) to minimize learning effects. The intermittent IHG exercise protocol with short exercise duration (EX) and control condition (CON) were performed on different days randomly to counterbalance across the subjects. On the day of the CON, each subject was asked to read a Japanese scientific magazine for the same experimental period as the EX (20 min) instead of performing the EX. On the day of the EX, each subject performed three repetitions of their MVC of the IHG exercise on the left hand (non-dominant hand) to determine the exercise intensity for the experiment. After a sufficient resting time (more than 30 min), each subject performed the EX, which consisted of sixteen 30-s isometric contractions at 30% of MVC separated by 45-s rest between IHG exercise trials with the non-dominant hand. In both conditions, before and after the EX and CON, the subjects performed the cognitive tasks. All studies were performed experiments on the same time (8AM–13PM) of day for both conditions at a constant temperature (24℃). In our pilot study [29], we confirmed that the response of the mean arterial pressure to the EX (intermittent IHG exercise protocol) was lower (99 ± 11 mmHg vs. 124 ± 8 mmHg, P = 0.007, n = 7) than that of the traditional IHG exercise protocol (four 2-min isometric contractions at 30% of MVC separated by 3 min of rest) used previous study [33].

Measurements

Cognitive performance (Go/No-Go tasks, memory recognition task)

Cognitive performance in this study was measured using the Go/No-Go tasks [34] and the memory recognition task [35], which require executive function and memory recognition, respectively. To evaluate cognitive performance, the mean reaction time (RT) and performance accuracy of the Go/No-Go task and memory recognition task were calculated.

Go/No-Go task: each trial started with a blank screen for 2.5 s, followed by a reparatory stimulus (green square) presentation at the center of the computer screen for 1 s. Then, one square of several possible colors (red, blue, yellow, or purple) was presented at the center of the computer display for 1 s. The red and blue squares represented the Go signal, and the yellow and purple squares represented the No-Go signal. If the Go signal was displayed, the subjects were asked to press the left button of the computer mouse with their right index finger as quickly as possible. If the No-Go signal was displayed, the participants were asked to refrain from responding. The Go/No-Go task consisted of 60 trials with equal probability (30 Go trials and 30 No-Go trials).

Memory recognition task: first, 30 Japanese words were presented at a rate of 1 word per 1 s for memorization. Approximately 5 min after the Go/No-Go task, the subjects performed the memory recognition portion of the test. To assess memory recognition performance, 60 words (30 distracters) were presented every 2 s, and at each word presentation, the subjects answered as quickly as possible whether that word was presented during the memorization period.

Cardiovascular measures

Heart rate (HR) was measured using a lead II electrocardiogram (bedside monitor, BMS-3400; Nihon Kohden, Tokyo, Japan). ABP was monitored continuously using a finger photoplethysmography (Finapres Medical Systems, Amsterdam, the Netherlands). Systolic blood pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP), and mean arterial pressure (MAP) were obtained from the ABP waveform. The HR, SBP, DBP, and MAP data were averaged using 30-s data points at the resting baseline and end of the intervention (last set). To identify the change in these parameters during EX, 30 s averages were calculated during the 4th, 8th and 12th sets of EX. The psychological arousal level was evaluated using the felt arousal scale (FAS; 1: low arousal to 6: high arousal) immediately after the cognitive tasks [36].

Statistical analysis

The values are expressed as the mean ± SD. Changes in HR, SBP, DBP, and MAP during EX were assessed with a one-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). Regarding the data of CON, changes in these values were assessed using a paired t-test because of two data points (the baseline and end of experimental period). Absolute changes in the RT and performance accuracy of the cognitive task and the arousal level during the Go/No-Go and memory tasks were assessed by a two-way (time: pre- and post-test × condition: CON and EX) repeated-measures ANOVA. The effect size were calculated as eta-squared (η2) for all ANOVA outcome or Hedges’ gav for t-test using the spreadsheet provided by Lakens [37]. The ANOVAs were followed by the Bonferroni's multiple post hoc test. A P-value < 0.05 was considered significant.

Results

The cardiovascular responses to the EX and CON are shown in Table 1. In the CON, the cardiovascular responses did not change from the baseline value (t21 > 0.359, P > 0.271, Hedges’ gav > 0.052). During the EX, HR did not increase (F1,21 = 1.246, P = 0.298, η2 = 0.010). Conversely, SBP, DBP, and MAP increased (F1,21 > 21.214, P < 0.001, η2 > 0.182). The psychological response during the cognitive tasks is shown in Table 2. The FAS remained unchanged during the cognitive tasks in the CON, but increased in the EX (F1,21 = 4.915, P = 0.038, η2 = 0.060); post hoc comparisons using the Bonferroni test indicated that the increase in the FAS during cognitive tasks was observed after the EX (P = 0.013). However, the EX did not change the RT or performance accuracy in the Go/No-Go task (F1,21 = 1.581, P = 0.222, η2 = 0.020 and F1,21 = 1.340, P = 0.260, η2 = 0.027, respectively, Fig. 1) or in the memory recognition task (F1,21 = 0.657, P = 0.427, η2 = 0.012 and F1,21 = 1.428, P = 0.245, η2 = 0.020, respectively, Fig. 2) despite an exercise-induced increase in the FAS.

Table 1 Cardiovascular measurement at resting baseline, end of the intervention (last set) and during intermittent isometric handgrip exercise protocol with short duration
Table 2 The psychological arousal level during the cognitive tasks
Fig. 1
figure 1

Reaction time (a) and performance accuracy (b) of the Go/No-Go task in the CON (control condition) and EX (intermittent IHG exercise protocol with short duration) (n = 22)

Fig. 2
figure 2

Reaction time (a) and performance accuracy (b) of memory recognition task in the CON (control condition) and EX (intermittent IHG exercise protocol with short duration) (n = 22)

Discussion

The intermittent IHG exercise protocol with short exercise duration, which minimizes the blood pressure response, did not improve cognitive performance despite causing an exercise-induced increase in the psychological arousal level. This intermittent IHG protocol is useful as a safe and effective exercise mode in the elderly and in patients with cardiovascular diseases who have a higher risk of cardiovascular events; however, this exercise stimulation is not sufficient for improving cognitive performance. To improve the clinical applicability of these findings, we must further investigate and identify an adequate mode of small muscle exercise that can be applied to safely improve cognitive performance for the elderly and patients with cardiovascular disease.

Some epidemiological longitudinal studies [38, 39] have reported that both dynamic and isometric exercise training (chronic exercise influence) improved cognitive performance, and thus confirm the validity of the findings presented in epidemiological cross-sectional study [40]. The mechanism of these exercise training-improved cognitive function remains unknown, but previous findings suggest that the physiological adaptation to exercise training, e.g., direct (brain volume) [41] and indirect (resting blood pressure, insulin resistance, physical fitness) physiological factors [38, 39, 42,43,44], is associated with the improvement in cognitive function.

On the other hand, investigating the effect of acute exercise (one bout of exercise) on cognitive performance can be isolated from the chronic exercise-induced effects and consequently provides important information regarding the acute influence of exercise on brain function because one bout of exercise does not alter brain volume, insulin resistance, physical fitness, etc. Under these backgrounds, many previous studies have investigated the acute effect of one bout of exercise on cognitive performance. One previous study [45] reported that dynamic exercise improves cognitive performance. The authors also manipulated cerebral blood flow, but changes in cerebral blood flow did not modify cognitive performance. Thus, the authors concluded from these results that acute exercise-stimulated neural activation rather than cerebral metabolism improved cognitive performance. Another study [46] demonstrated that cognitive performance was improved after acute high-intensity interval training, and this improvement is associated with exercise-induced lactate production. It is noteworthy that these acute exercise-induced improvements in cognitive function occur without a chronic physiological adaptation, for example, caused by exercise training.

Conversely, the acute effect of small muscle exercises on cognitive performance is poorly understood. Our recent study [25] demonstrated that the traditional IHG exercise protocol improved cognitive performance. In addition, a previous study reported that an acute dynamic exercise-induced increase in arousal level was associated with increased brain neural activity and cognitive performance [47]. However, in contrast to these previous studies and our hypothesis, the intermittent IHG exercise protocol with short exercise duration did not improve cognitive performance despite an increase in the FAS (arousal level). This finding highlights two important points in this research area. First, small muscle exercise may be different from large muscle exercise with regard to improvement in cognitive performance. One previous study [48] that reported on an acute isometric exercise using a larger muscle (leg extension–quadriceps femoris muscle), compared with IHG exercise, noted improved cognitive performance. Thus, small muscle exercises may be limited in their ability to improve cognitive performance. Indeed, the increase in the FAS during exercise in the present study was lower than that during the large muscle isometric exercise (0.5 vs 1–2) [48]. Second, we used the intermittent IHG exercise protocol with short exercise duration to prevent a large blood pressure response, and during this exercise protocol, the HR sample entropy was larger than that of the traditional IHG exercise protocol, which included longer contractions (2-min at 30% MVC IHG and 1-min recovery × 4 trials, corresponding to the same amount of exercise as the intermittent IHG exercise protocol in this study) [28]. These findings indicate that the intermittent IHG exercise protocol in the present study provided a lower sympathetic stimulus and thus autonomic stimulation may be not enough to improve cognitive performance. Importantly, our previous study found that the traditional acute IHG exercise protocol (2-min at 25% MVC IHG and 3-min recovery × 4 trials) increased blood pressure to a larger extent and improved cognitive performance [25]. Thus, the IHG exercise may be a useful exercise mode to improve cognitive performance, but we must consider an adequate exercise protocol (strength, duration, frequency and so on) using IHG to safely improve cognitive performance. To perform this rehabilitation exercise in the elderly and in patients with cardiovascular diseases, we must further investigate and establish an adequate IHG exercise protocol based on the balance between sufficient autonomic stimulation and preventing a large blood pressure response.

Potential limitations of the present study should be considered. Since we recruited only young healthy adults, the present results cannot be generalized to older or hypertensive individuals. However, given that exercise training-induced cardiovascular adaptations improve cognitive performance, older and hypertensive individuals may gain a large cognitive improvement through exercise therapy. Further investigations regarding this point in the clinical research area must be performed.

Conclusions

In contrast to our hypothesis, the intermittent IHG exercise protocol with short exercise duration did not improve cognitive performance despite causing an exercise-induced increase in the psychological arousal level. To improve the clinical applicability of these findings, we must further investigate and establish an adequate IHG exercise protocol based on the balance between sufficient autonomic stimulation and preventing a large blood pressure response.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and analyzed during current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Fiocco AJ, Yaffe K (2010) Defining successful aging: the importance of including cognitive function over time. Arch Neurol 67(7):876–880

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Logsdon RG, McCurry SM, Teri L (2007) Evidence-based interventions to improve quality of life for individuals with dementia. Alzheimers Care Today 8(4):309–318

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Breteler MM, Claus JJ, Grobbee DE, Hofman A (1994) Cardiovascular disease and distribution of cognitive function in elderly people: the Rotterdam Study. BMJ 308(6944):1604–1608

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Kilander L, Nyman H, Boberg M, Hansson L, Lithell H (1998) Hypertension is related to cognitive impairment: a 20-year follow-up of 999 men. Hypertension 31(3):780–786

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Song D, Yu DS, Li PW, He G, Sun Q (2019) Correlates of health-related quality of life among chinese older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Clin Interv Aging 14:2205–2212

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Alghadir AH, Gabr SA, Al-Momani M, Al-Momani F (2020) Moderate aerobic training modulates cytokines and cortisol profiles in older adults with cognitive abilities. Cytokine 138:155373

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Dempster DW, Brown JP, Fahrleitner-Pammer A, Kendler D, Rizzo S, Valter I, Wagman RB, Yin X, Yue SV, Boivin G (2018) Effects of long-term denosumab on bone histomorphometry and mineralization in women with postmenopausal osteoporosis. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 103(7):2498–2509

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Weuve J, Kang JH, Manson JE, Breteler MM, Ware JH, Grodstein F (2004) Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA 292(12):1454–1461

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Whelton SP, Chin A, Xin X, He J (2002) Effect of aerobic exercise on blood pressure: a meta-analysis of randomized, controlled trials. Ann Intern Med 136(7):493–503

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Delaney EP, Greaney JL, Edwards DG, Rose WC, Fadel PJ, Farquhar WB (2010) Exaggerated sympathetic and pressor responses to handgrip exercise in older hypertensive humans: role of the muscle metaboreflex. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol 299(5):H1318-1327

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Middlekauff HR, Nitzsche EU, Hoh CK, Hamilton MA, Fonarow GC, Hage A, Moriguchi JD (2000) Exaggerated renal vasoconstriction during exercise in heart failure patients. Circulation 101(7):784–789

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Muller MD, Drew RC, Blaha CA, Mast JL, Cui J, Reed AB, Sinoway LI (2012) Oxidative stress contributes to the augmented exercise pressor reflex in peripheral arterial disease patients. J Physiol 590(23):6237–6246

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Negrao CE, Rondon MU, Tinucci T, Alves MJ, Roveda F, Braga AM, Reis SF, Nastari L, Barretto AC, Krieger EM et al (2001) Abnormal neurovascular control during exercise is linked to heart failure severity. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol 280(3):H1286-1292

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Piepoli M, Clark AL, Volterrani M, Adamopoulos S, Sleight P, Coats AJ (1996) Contribution of muscle afferents to the hemodynamic, autonomic, and ventilatory responses to exercise in patients with chronic heart failure: effects of physical training. Circulation 93(5):940–952

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Fisher JP, Ogoh S, Ahmed A, Aro MR, Gute D, Fadel PJ (2007) Influence of age on cardiac baroreflex function during dynamic exercise in humans. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol 293(1):H777-783

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Lind AR, McNicol GW (1967) Muscular factors which determine the cardiovascular responses to sustained and rhythmic exercise. Can Med Assoc J 96(12):706–715

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Inder JD, Carlson DJ, Dieberg G, McFarlane JR, Hess NC, Smart NA (2016) Isometric exercise training for blood pressure management: a systematic review and meta-analysis to optimize benefit. Hypertens Res 39(2):88–94

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Cornelissen VA, Smart NA (2013) Exercise training for blood pressure: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Am Heart Assoc 2(1):e004473

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Loaiza-Betancur AF, Chulvi-Medrano I (2020) Is low-intensity isometric handgrip exercise an efficient alternative in lifestyle blood pressure management? a systematic review. Sports Health 12(5):470–477

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Taylor AC, McCartney N, Kamath MV, Wiley RL (2003) Isometric training lowers resting blood pressure and modulates autonomic control. Med Sci Sports Exerc 35(2):251–256

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Cahu Rodrigues SL, Farah BQ, Silva G, Correia M, Pedrosa R, Vianna L, Ritti-Dias RM (2020) Vascular effects of isometric handgrip training in hypertensives. Clin Exp Hypertens 42(1):24–30

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kelley GA, Kelley KS (2010) Isometric handgrip exercise and resting blood pressure: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Hypertens 28(3):411–418

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Owen A, Wiles J, Swaine I (2010) Effect of isometric exercise on resting blood pressure: a meta-analysis. J Hum Hypertens 24(12):796–800

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Yamagata T, Sako T (2020) High cardiovascular reactivity and muscle strength attenuate hypotensive effects of isometric handgrip training in young women: a randomized controlled trial. Clin Exp Hypertens 42(7):595–600

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Washio T, Suzuki K, Saito S, Watanabe H, Ando S, Brothers RM, Ogoh S (2021) Effects of acute interval handgrip exercise on cognitive performance. Physiol Behav 232:113327

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Smolander J, Aminoff T, Korhonen I, Tervo M, Shen N, Korhonen O, Louhevaara V (1998) Heart rate and blood pressure responses to isometric exercise in young and older men. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol 77(5):439–444

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Masaki M, Mitchell JH, Smith SA (2016) The exercise pressor reflex in hypertension. J Phys Fitness Sports Med 5(5):339–347

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Millar PJ, MacDonald MJ, McCartney N (2011) Effects of isometric handgrip protocol on blood pressure and neurocardiac modulation. Int J Sports Med 32(3):174–180

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Saito S, Washio T, Watanabe H, Tamiya K, Yamada H, Ando S, Ogoh S (2020) Effect of different interval handgrip protocol on cognitive performance. J Phys Fitness Sports Med 9(6):292

    Google Scholar 

  30. Sugimoto T, Suga T, Tsukamoto H, Tomoo K, Dora K, Hashimoto T, Isaka T (2020) Effect of repeated bouts versus a single bout of moderate-intensity exercise on postexercise inhibitory control. Physiol Rep 8(15):e14528

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Ichinose Y, Morishita S, Suzuki R, Endo G, Tsubaki A (2020) Comparison of the effects of continuous and intermittent exercise on cerebral oxygenation and cognitive function. Adv Exp Med Biol 1232:209–214

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Tomoo K, Suga T, Sugimoto T, Tanaka D, Shimoho K, Dora K, Mok E, Matsumoto S, Tsukamoto H, Takada S et al (2020) Work volume is an important variable in determining the degree of inhibitory control improvements following resistance exercise. Physiol Rep 8(15):e14527

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Wiley RL, Dunn CL, Cox RH, Hueppchen NA, Scott MS (1992) Isometric exercise training lowers resting blood pressure. Med Sci Sports Exerc 24(7):749–754

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Akagi R, Tonotsuka M, Horie R, Hirata K, Ando S (2019) Effect of acute eye fatigue on cognition for young females: a pilot study. PeerJ 7:e7978

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Lefferts WK, Babcock MC, Tiss MJ, Ives SJ, White CN, Brutsaert TD, Heffernan KS (2016) Effect of hypoxia on cerebrovascular and cognitive function during moderate intensity exercise. Physiol Behav 165:108–118

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Svebak S, Murgatroyd S (1985) Metamotivational dominance: a multimethod validation of reversal theory constructs. J Pers Soc Psychol 48(1):107–116

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Lakens D (2013) Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front Psychol 4:863

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Kleinloog JPD, Mensink RP, Ivanov D, Adam JJ, Uludag K, Joris PJ (2019) Aerobic exercise training improves cerebral blood flow and executive function: a randomized, controlled cross-over trial in sedentary older men. Front Aging Neurosci 11:333

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Dempster K, McGowan CL, Wade TJ, O’Leary D (2018) Effects of isometric handgrip exercise training on systemic arterial stiffness, cardiovagal baroreflex sensitivity, and cognition in adults with hypertension: a pilot study. Crit Rev Phys Rehabil Med 30(3):219–237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Middleton LE, Barnes DE, Lui LY, Yaffe K (2010) Physical activity over the life course and its association with cognitive performance and impairment in old age. J Am Geriatr Soc 58(7):1322–1326

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, Kim JS, Heo S, Alves H, White SM et al (2011) Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108(7):3017–3022

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Hajjar I, Hart M, Mack W, Lipsitz LA (2015) Aldosterone, cognitive function, and cerebral hemodynamics in hypertension and antihypertensive therapy. Am J Hypertens 28(3):319–325

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Vincent C, Hall PA (2015) Executive function in adults with type 2 diabetes: a meta-analytic review. Psychosom Med 77(6):631–642

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Satariano WA, Tager IB (2003) A longitudinal study of cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive function in healthy older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 51(4):459–465

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Ogoh S, Tsukamoto H, Hirasawa A, Hasegawa H, Hirose N, Hashimoto T (2014) The effect of changes in cerebral blood flow on cognitive function during exercise. Physiol Rep 2(9):e12163

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Tsukamoto H, Suga T, Takenaka S, Tanaka D, Takeuchi T, Hamaoka T, Isaka T, Ogoh S, Hashimoto T (2016) Repeated high-intensity interval exercise shortens the positive effect on executive function during post-exercise recovery in healthy young males. Physiol Behav 160:26–34

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Byun K, Hyodo K, Suwabe K, Ochi G, Sakairi Y, Kato M, Dan I, Soya H (2014) Positive effect of acute mild exercise on executive function via arousal-related prefrontal activations: an fNIRS study. Neuroimage 98:336–345

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Tsukamoto H, Suga T, Takenaka S, Takeuchi T, Tanaka D, Hamaoka T, Hashimoto T, Isaka T (2017) An acute bout of localized resistance exercise can rapidly improve inhibitory control. PLoS ONE 12(9):e0184075

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors appreciate the time and effort expended by the volunteer subjects and staffs.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship and/or publication of this article: this study was in part supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Japanese Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (grant number 15H003098).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SS, SO: conception and design of research; SS, TW, HW, SA, SO performed experiments; SS, TW, HW analyzed data; SS, SO, SA interpreted results of experiments; SS prepared figures; SS, SO drafted manuscript. All authors edited and revised manuscript. All authors approved final version of manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shigehiko Ogoh.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Experimental protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Toyo University (Approval Number: TU2019-039).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Saito, S., Washio, T., Watanabe, H. et al. Effect of intermittent isometric handgrip exercise protocol with short exercise duration on cognitive performance. J Physiol Sci 71, 12 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12576-021-00796-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12576-021-00796-z

Keywords