Skip to main content
  • Mini-review
  • Published:

Protective and therapeutic effects of exercise on stress-induced memory impairment

Abstract

The objective of this paper was to systematically evaluate the potential preventive and therapeutic effects of exercise in attenuating stress-induced memory impairment. A systematic review was employed, searching PubMed, PsychInfo, Sports Discus and Google Scholar databases. For eligibility, studies had to be published in English, employ an experimental design, have the acute or chronic bout of exercise occur prior to, during or after the stressor, implement a psychophysiological stressor, and have an assessment of memory function occurring after the stressor. In total, 23 studies were evaluated, all of which were conducted among animal models. All 23 studies employed a chronic exercise protocol and a chronic stress protocol. Eight studies evaluated a preventive model, three employed a concurrent model, ten studies employed a therapeutic model, and two studies evaluated both a preventive and therapeutic model within the same study. Among the eight studies employing a preventive model, all eight demonstrated that the stress regimen impaired memory function. In all eight of these studies, when exercise occurred prior to the stressor, exercise attenuated the stress-induced memory impairment effect. Among the ten studies employing a therapeutic model, one study showed that the stress protocol enhanced memory function, one showed that the stress protocol did not influence memory, and eight demonstrated that the stress regimen impaired memory function. Among the eight studies showing that the stress protocol impaired memory function, all eight studies demonstrated that exercise, after the stressor, attenuated stress-induced memory impairment. Within animal models, chronic stress is associated with memory impairment and chronic exercise has both a preventive and therapeutic effect in attenuating stress-induced memory impairment. Additional experimental work in human studies is needed. Such work should also examine acute exercise and stress protocols.

Introduction

The prophylactic and treatment effects of exercise on various chronic diseases is well established [1]. Additionally, exercise can also help to prevent a host of cardiometabolic-related conditions (e.g., diabetes, early mortality) [2, 3]. Emerging work also demonstrates that, exercise, prior to a psychophysiological stressor [herein focused on toxic stress (not eustress)] [4], can mitigate the negative effects of the stressor. For example, we showed that acute exercise, occurring immediately before viewing emotionally charged, negatively valenced images, helped facilitate emotional regulation [5]. This “exercise preventive paradigm” has also been corroborated with other emotional regulation studies [6, 7].

This exercise preventive paradigm effect may also have implications in memory function. Psychophysiological stressors, such as forced social participation in a verbal presentation task, may have a negative effect on memory function [8,9,10,11,12,13,14]. Notably, this stress-memory relationship is thought to follow an inverted U-shaped relationship [15]. See Fig. 1 (and the Discussion section) for a schematic on the potential underlying mechanisms through which stress (both acute and chronic) may influence memory function. To our knowledge, however, there are no published reviews comprehensively evaluating the literature regarding the potential protective and/or therapeutic effects of exercise on mitigating stress-induced memory impairment [16], which was the purpose of this brief systematic review. The plausibility for exercise to attenuate stress-induced memory impairment is also shown in Fig. 1 (and further addressed in the Discussion section).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Schematic indicating potential mechanisms through which acute stress may enhance memory, detrimental effects of chronic stress on memory, and how exercise may attenuate stress-induced memory impairment. The circular crossed symbol denotes that chronic exercise attenuates the chronic stress mechanisms

Methods

Studies were identified using electronic databases, including PubMed, PsychInfo, Sports Discus, and Google Scholar. We employed the computerized searches on April 25, 2018, identifying articles published prior to this date (no restriction was placed on how far back the study was published). The search terms included exercise, physical activity, stress, psychophysiological, rescue, preventive, treatment, therapeutic, psychological, memory, and cognition. To be eligible for inclusion in this systematic review, studies had to be published in English, employ an experimental design (cross-sectional designs on this topic were not eligible) [17], have the acute or chronic bout of exercise occur prior to, during, or after the stressor, implement a toxic psychophysiological stressor (pharmaceutical agent or ischemia-induction were not eligible [18,19,20,21,22], mild forms of the stressor were not eligible [23], and evaluating individuals without experimentally manipulating stress were not eligible) [24], and have an assessment of memory function that occurred after the stressor. To provide a comprehensive assessment on this topic, we applied no restriction on whether the study was conducted in humans or animal models. In total, 24 studies met our criteria. However, two appeared to be duplicate studies [25, 26], and thus, one was removed. As noted in Table 1, 23 studies were evaluated herein.

Table 1 Extraction table of the evaluated studies

Results

Table 1 displays the extraction table for the evaluated studies. All were experimental studies conducted in an animal model. The stress protocol across the studies varied, including maternal separation, loud noise exposure, immobilization/restraint, social defeat/competition and exposure to cat odor. All studies, except one (single session acute stress protocol), employed a chronic stress protocol (e.g., multiple repeated exposures over 1–2 weeks). All 23 studies employed a chronic exercise protocol (e.g., daily exercise from 2 to 8 weeks; either forced treadmill exercise or voluntary wheel access). Among the 23 studies, the commonly assessed memory tasks included the Morris water maze, object recognition test, or inhibitory avoidance task.

Eight studies evaluated a preventive model (i.e., exercise occurring prior to stress-induction), three employed a concurrent model (exercise bout occurred during or around the same time as the stress protocol), ten studies employed a therapeutic model (i.e., exercise occurring after stress-induction), and two studies evaluated both a preventive and therapeutic model within the same study.

Among the eight studies employing a preventive model, all eight demonstrated that the stress regimen impaired memory function. In all eight of these studies, when exercise occurred prior to the stressor, exercise attenuated the stress-induced memory impairment effect.

Among the ten studies employing a therapeutic model, one study showed that the stress protocol enhanced memory function, one showed that the stress protocol did not influence memory, and eight demonstrated that the stress regimen impaired memory function. Among the eight studies showing that the stress protocol impaired memory function, all eight studies demonstrated that exercise, after the stressor, attenuated stress-induced memory impairment.

Among the three concurrent models, and the two studies that evaluated both preventive and therapeutic effects, all showed that the stress protocol impaired memory function. Among the three concurrent models, two demonstrated a beneficial effect of exercise in mitigating stress-induced memory impairment. Among the two studies employing both a preventive and therapeutic model, both demonstrated attenuation effects of exercise on stress-induced memory impairment.

Discussion

The objective of this systematic review was to evaluate the potential preventive and therapeutic effects of exercise in attenuating stress-induced memory impairment. There was consistent evidence that chronic exercise had both a preventive and therapeutic effect in mitigating chronic stress-induced memory impairment. The narrative that follows will discuss these mechanistic pathways, as displayed in Fig. 1. For additional discussion on these mechanisms, the reader is referred elsewhere [16, 27, 28].

Acute stress and memory

Acute moderate levels of stress may enhance memory, particularly emotional-based information (vs neutral stimuli). Specifically, enhanced encoding and consolidation of stimuli is more likely to occur for information perceived as “high priority” [29,30,31,32,33]. The stressor, occurring prior to the memory task, may help to augment attentional resources (via, for example, the prefrontal and parietal structures) to the memory stimuli and, in turn, enhance encoding of the information [34, 35]. In addition to psychological stress, emerging work also suggests that exercise-induced physiological arousal may help to subserve stress-related memory function (emotional memory) [36]. Additional work is needed to determine whether there is an additive effect of exercise and acute stress on memory function.

Additionally, the stressor (including exercise) [36], occurring before, or shortly after, the memory task can help to facilitate the consolidation of the memory trace. For example, cortisol crosses the blood–brain barrier and binds to mineralocorticoid or glucocorticoid receptors. After which, PKA activation may help to facilitate exocytosis of AMPA receptors (and activation of NMDA receptors) [37], subserving hippocampal LTP [38]. Acute stress may also induce levels of epinephrine, activating the vagus nerve and, in turn, facilitating LTP via neurotransmitter (e.g., norepinephrine, dopamine, serotonin, and acetylcholine) production to the hippocampus [39,40,41,42]. To illustrate, the vagus nerve may stimulate the production of norepinephrine from the locus coeruleus, which then binds to adrenergic receptors, ultimately facilitating a cascade of intracellular signaling to induce synaptic plasticity [43]. Moreover, cortisol may augment endocannabinoid levels, binding to CB1 receptors in GABA interneurons and, ultimately, inhibiting GABA neurotransmitter levels [44]. This, in turn, may help to preserve memory, as GABA inhibition may help facilitate LTP [45] and GABA receptor activation may impair memory [46].

Although acute stress, occurring before encoding or during the early stages of consolidation, can facilitate encoding and consolidation of the prioritized stimuli, it can have the opposite effect on non-prioritized stimuli. The encoding of the stressful event may compete with the encoding of non-relevant or non-prioritized stimuli. Further, if the stressor occurs around the period of retrieving a memory, this memory retrieval process can be impaired, as attentional resources are shifted away from retrieval processes to encoding the stressful event. Additionally, inactive synapses, representing previously encoded stimuli, may become de-potentiated when LTP increases in other synapses [28]. Moreover, during the stressor, reduced neuronal firing may occur in the prefrontal cortex, which may impair memory retrieval since the prefrontal cortex plays an important role in such retrieval processes [47]. It would be worthwhile to investigate whether acute exercise can attenuate these effects by, for example, attenuating the stress response and facilitating emotional regulation [5].

Taken together, acute moderate levels of stress may help to facilitate encoding and consolidation of prioritized stimuli (particularly emotional stimuli), whereas extreme acute stressors may detrimentally influence retrieval of memories when the stressor occurs around the time of retrieving an unrelated memory. Notably, and as discussed next, chronic elevations in cortisol, lasting more than a few hours, can impair memory function (inducing LTD) [38].

Chronic stress and memory

Chronic stress may detrimentally influence stress through various mechanisms, including enhanced HPA axis activity. Over time, this may impair cell survival and neuronal morphology (e.g., loss of spines, shrinkage of dendrites) [4]. Regarding cell survival, astrocytes, which support the survival of neurons, possess glucocorticoid receptors and are significantly affected by chronic psychosocial stress [48]. Considering neuronal morphology, reduced synaptic firing, via LTD for example, causes actin loss and dendritic spine shrinkage [49]. Further, chronic stress may reduce BDNF levels [50], which play an important role in facilitating signaling pathways (e.g., RAC1) that stabilize dendritic spines [51]. Additionally, chronic stress may reduce neurotransmitter levels (e.g., dopamine) [52], decrease AMPA receptor expression [53], and downregulate mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptor expression [54]. This downregulation and desensitization of these receptors may prevent activation of some of the above-mentioned cellular pathways (e.g., PKA) that may facilitate LTP. Further, chronic stress may inhibit neurogenesis, and ultimately, hippocampal volume loss, via, for example, apoptosis of progenitor cells to cell cycle arrest [49].

Exercise mitigates negative effects of chronic stress

Exercise may attenuate the memory-related consequences of chronic stress via various pathways. Ultimately, exercise may help to facilitate LTP through induced neuronal excitability, via stimulation of the vagus nerve as well as muscle afferent nerve fibers [55], which have direct projections to the brainstem and, ultimately, the hippocampus. Further, exercise-induced alterations in hormones (e.g., epinephrine and cortisol) can also influence neuronal excitability. Facilitating these effects, exercise has been shown to enhance neurotrophic factors (e.g., BDNF), induce transcription factors (e.g., CREB) expression, and increase AMPA trafficking [55]. Exercise may also help attenuate chronic stress-induced memory impairment via attenuation of HPA axis activity, suppress oxidative stress, facilitate neurogenesis, and regulate mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptor expression [27].

Conclusion

This review demonstrates that, within animal models, chronic stress is associated with memory impairment and chronic exercise has both a preventive and therapeutic effect in attenuating stress-induced memory impairment. Given the paucity of work among human studies, future work on this topic among humans should investigate, specifically, whether exercise has a preventive and therapeutic effect in mitigating memory impairment caused from psychophysiological stress. Such work should also consider models that evaluate acute exercise and acute stress protocols. Further, work should also evaluate varying parameters of exercise, such as the intensity, duration, and type of exercise, as variations of these dimensions may have a unique influence on potentially attenuating stress-induced memory impairment.

References

  1. Kujala UM (2009) Evidence on the effects of exercise therapy in the treatment of chronic disease. Br J Sports Med 43(8):550–555. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2009.059808

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Warburton DE, Nicol CW, Bredin SS (2006) Health benefits of physical activity: the evidence. CMAJ 174(6):801–809. https://doi.org/10.1503/cmaj.051351

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Warburton DER, Bredin SSD (2017) Health benefits of physical activity: a systematic review of current systematic reviews. Curr Opin Cardiol 32(5):541–556. https://doi.org/10.1097/HCO.0000000000000437

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. McEwen BS (2017) Neurobiological and systemic effects of chronic stress. Chronic Stress (Thousand Oaks). https://doi.org/10.1177/2470547017692328

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Edwards MK, Rhodes RE, Loprinzi PD (2017) A randomized control intervention investigating the effects of acute exercise on emotional regulation. Am J Health Behav 41(5):534–543. https://doi.org/10.5993/AJHB.41.5.2

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Bernstein EE, McNally RJ (2017) Acute aerobic exercise helps overcome emotion regulation deficits. Cogn Emot 31(4):834–843. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1168284

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Bernstein EE, McNally RJ (2017) Acute aerobic exercise hastens emotional recovery from a subsequent stressor. Health Psychol 36(6):560–567. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000482

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. de Quervain DJ, Henke K, Aerni A et al (2003) Glucocorticoid-induced impairment of declarative memory retrieval is associated with reduced blood flow in the medial temporal lobe. Eur J Neurosci 17(6):1296–1302

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. de Quervain DJ, Roozendaal B, Nitsch RM et al (2000) Acute cortisone administration impairs retrieval of long-term declarative memory in humans. Nat Neurosci 3(4):313–314. https://doi.org/10.1038/73873

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Het S, Ramlow G, Wolf OT (2005) A meta-analytic review of the effects of acute cortisol administration on human memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30(8):771–784. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.03.005

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. de Quervain DJ, Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL (1998) Stress and glucocorticoids impair retrieval of long-term spatial memory. Nature 394(6695):787–790. https://doi.org/10.1038/29542

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Diamond DM, Park CR, Heman KL et al (1999) Exposing rats to a predator impairs spatial working memory in the radial arm water maze. Hippocampus 9(5):542–552. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1999)9:5%3c542:AID-HIPO8%3e3.0.CO;2-N

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Park CR, Zoladz PR, Conrad CD et al (2008) Acute predator stress impairs the consolidation and retrieval of hippocampus-dependent memory in male and female rats. Learn Mem 15(4):271–280. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.721108

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Guez J, Saar-Ashkenazy R, Keha E et al (2016) The effect of Trier social stress test (TSST) on item and associative recognition of words and pictures in healthy participants. Front Psychol 7:507. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00507

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Salehi B, Cordero MI, Sandi C (2010) Learning under stress: the inverted-U-shape function revisited. Learn Mem 17(10):522–530. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.1914110

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Leem YH (2017) The potential role of exercise in chronic stress-related changes in AMPA receptor phenotype underlying synaptic plasticity. J Exerc Nutr Biochem 21(4):11–15. https://doi.org/10.20463/jenb.2017.0037

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Head D, Singh T, Bugg JM (2012) The moderating role of exercise on stress-related effects on the hippocampus and memory in later adulthood. Neuropsychology 26(2):133–143. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027108

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Schimidt HL, Vieira A, Altermann C et al (2014) Memory deficits and oxidative stress in cerebral ischemia-reperfusion: neuroprotective role of physical exercise and green tea supplementation. Neurobiol Learn Mem 114:242–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2014.07.005

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Shih PC, Yang YR, Wang RY (2013) Effects of exercise intensity on spatial memory performance and hippocampal synaptic plasticity in transient brain ischemic rats. PLoS One 8(10):e78163. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078163

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Wang Q, Xu Z, Tang J et al (2013) Voluntary exercise counteracts Abeta25-35-induced memory impairment in mice. Behav Brain Res 256:618–625. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.09.024

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Cechetti F, Worm PV, Elsner VR et al (2012) Forced treadmill exercise prevents oxidative stress and memory deficits following chronic cerebral hypoperfusion in the rat. Neurobiol Learn Mem 97(1):90–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2011.09.008

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Gozal D, Nair D, Goldbart AD (2010) Physical activity attenuates intermittent hypoxia-induced spatial learning deficits and oxidative stress. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 182(1):104–112. https://doi.org/10.1164/rccm.201001-0108OC

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Adlard PA, Engesser-Cesar C, Cotman CW (2011) Mild stress facilitates learning and exercise improves retention in aged mice. Exp Gerontol 46(1):53–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.exger.2010.10.001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Zlomuzica A, Woud ML, Machulska A et al (2018) Deficits in episodic memory and mental time travel in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 83:42–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.12.014

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Radahmadi M, Alaei H, Sharifi MR et al (2013) The effect of synchronized running activity with chronic stress on passive avoidance learning and body weight in rats. Int J Prev Med 4(4):430–437

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Radahmadi M, Alaei H, Sharifi MR et al (2013) The effect of synchronized forced running with chronic stress on short, mid and long-term memory in rats. Asian J Sports Med 4(1):54–62

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Chen C, Nakagawa S, An Y et al (2017) The exercise-glucocorticoid paradox: how exercise is beneficial to cognition, mood, and the brain while increasing glucocorticoid levels. Front Neuroendocrinol 44:83–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2016.12.001

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Gagnon SA, Wagner AD (2016) Acute stress and episodic memory retrieval: neurobiological mechanisms and behavioral consequences. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1369(1):55–75. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12996

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Clewett DV, Huang R, Velasco R et al (2018) Locus coeruleus activity strengthens prioritized memories under arousal. J Neurosci 38(6):1558–1574. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2097-17.2017

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Clewett D, Sakaki M, Huang R et al (2017) Arousal amplifies biased competition between high and low priority memories more in women than in men: the role of elevated noradrenergic activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 80:80–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.02.022

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Lee TH, Itti L, Mather M (2012) Evidence for arousal-biased competition in perceptual learning. Front Psychol 3:241. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00241

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Lee TH, Sakaki M, Cheng R et al (2014) Emotional arousal amplifies the effects of biased competition in the brain. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9(12):2067–2077. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu015

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Mather M, Sutherland MR (2011) Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspect Psychol Sci 6(2):114–133. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611400234

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Dehaene S, Kerszberg M, Changeux JP (1998) A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95(24):14529–14534

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Daffner KR, Scinto LF, Weitzman AM et al (2003) Frontal and parietal components of a cerebral network mediating voluntary attention to novel events. J Cogn Neurosci 15(2):294–313

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Loprinzi PD, Frith E, Edwards MK (2018) Exercise and emotional memory: a systematic review. J Cogn Enhanc. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-018-0086-z

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Whitehead G, Jo J, Hogg EL et al (2013) Acute stress causes rapid synaptic insertion of Ca2+-permeable AMPA receptors to facilitate long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Brain 136(Pt 12):3753–3765. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt293

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Joels M, Krugers HJ (2007) LTP after stress: up or down? Neural Plast 2007:93202. https://doi.org/10.1155/2007/93202

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Marzo A, Bai J, Otani S (2009) Neuroplasticity regulation by noradrenaline in mammalian brain. Curr Neuropharmacol 7(4):286–295. https://doi.org/10.2174/157015909790031193

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Morcom AM, Bullmore ET, Huppert FA et al (2010) Memory encoding and dopamine in the aging brain: a psychopharmacological neuroimaging study. Cereb Cortex 20(3):743–757. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp139

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Mlinar B, Stocca G, Corradetti R (2015) Endogenous serotonin facilitates hippocampal long-term potentiation at CA3/CA1 synapses. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 122(2):177–185. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-014-1246-7

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Hasselmo ME (2006) The role of acetylcholine in learning and memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16(6):710–715. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2006.09.002

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Tully K, Bolshakov VY (2010) Emotional enhancement of memory: how norepinephrine enables synaptic plasticity. Mol Brain 3:15. https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-6606-3-15

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Hill MN, McEwen BS (2010) Involvement of the endocannabinoid system in the neurobehavioural effects of stress and glucocorticoids. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 34(5):791–797. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.11.001

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Chapman CA, Perez Y, Lacaille JC (1998) Effects of GABA(A) inhibition on the expression of long-term potentiation in CA1 pyramidal cells are dependent on tetanization parameters. Hippocampus 8(3):289–298. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:3%3c289:AID-HIPO10%3e3.0.CO;2-X

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Krebs-Kraft DL, Wheeler MG, Parent MB (2007) The memory-impairing effects of septal GABA receptor activation involve GABAergic septo-hippocampal projection neurons. Learn Mem 14(12):833–841. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.809407

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Preston AR, Eichenbaum H (2013) Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in memory. Curr Biol 23(17):R764–773. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.041

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Czeh B, Simon M, Schmelting B et al (2006) Astroglial plasticity in the hippocampus is affected by chronic psychosocial stress and concomitant fluoxetine treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology 31(8):1616–1626. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300982

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Krugers HJ, Lucassen PJ, Karst H et al (2010) Chronic stress effects on hippocampal structure and synaptic function: relevance for depression and normalization by anti-glucocorticoid treatment. Front Synaptic Neurosci 2:24. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00024

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Murakami S, Imbe H, Morikawa Y et al (2005) Chronic stress, as well as acute stress, reduces BDNF mRNA expression in the rat hippocampus but less robustly. Neurosci Res 53(2):129–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2005.06.008

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Koleske AJ (2013) Molecular mechanisms of dendrite stability. Nat Rev Neurosci 14(8):536–550. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3486

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Alghasham A, Rasheed N (2014) Stress-mediated modulations in dopaminergic system and their subsequent impact on behavioral and oxidative alterations: an update. Pharm Biol 52:368–377

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Kallarackal AJ, Kvarta MD, Cammarata E et al (2013) Chronic stress induces a selective decrease in AMPA receptor-mediated synaptic excitation at hippocampal temporoammonic-CA1 synapses. J Neurosci 33(40):15669–15674. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2588-13.2013

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Meyer U, van Kampen M, Isovich E et al (2001) Chronic psychosocial stress regulates the expression of both GR and MR mRNA in the hippocampal formation of tree shrews. Hippocampus 11(3):329–336. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.1047

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Loprinzi PD, Edwards MK, Frith E (2017) Potential avenues for exercise to activate episodic memory-related pathways: a narrative review. Eur J Neurosci 46(5):2067–2077. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13644

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Grace L, Hescham S, Kellaway LA et al (2009) Effect of exercise on learning and memory in a rat model of developmental stress. Metab Brain Dis 24(4):643–657. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11011-009-9162-5

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Mello PB, Benetti F, Cammarota M et al (2009) Physical exercise can reverse the deficit in fear memory induced by maternal deprivation. Neurobiol Learn Mem 92(3):364–369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2009.04.004

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Makena N, Bugarith K, Russell VA (2012) Maternal separation enhances object location memory and prevents exercise-induced MAPK/ERK signalling in adult Sprague-Dawley rats. Metab Brain Dis 27(3):377–385. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11011-012-9298-6

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Kim TW, Shin MS, Park JK et al (2013) Treadmill exercise alleviates prenatal noise stress-induced impairment of spatial learning ability through enhancing hippocampal neurogenesis in rat pups. J Exerc Rehabil 9(5):451–456. https://doi.org/10.12965/jer.130064

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Kim BK, Seo JH (2013) Treadmill exercise alleviates post-traumatic stress disorder-induced impairment of spatial learning memory in rats. J Exerc Rehabil 9(4):413–419. https://doi.org/10.12965/jer.130058

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Castilla-Ortega E, Rosell-Valle C, Pedraza C et al (2014) Voluntary exercise followed by chronic stress strikingly increases mature adult-born hippocampal neurons and prevents stress-induced deficits in ‘what-when-where’ memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem 109:62–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2013.12.001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Patki G, Solanki N, Atrooz F et al (2014) Novel mechanistic insights into treadmill exercise based rescue of social defeat-induced anxiety-like behavior and memory impairment in rats. Physiol Behav 130:135–144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.04.011

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Patki G, Li L, Allam F et al (2014) Moderate treadmill exercise rescues anxiety and depression-like behavior as well as memory impairment in a rat model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Physiol Behav 130:47–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.03.016

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Neves BH, Menezes J, Souza MA et al (2015) Physical exercise prevents short and long-term deficits on aversive and recognition memory and attenuates brain oxidative damage induced by maternal deprivation. Physiol Behav 152(Pt A):99–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.09.019

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Dief AE, Samy DM, Dowedar FI (2015) Impact of exercise and vitamin B1 intake on hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor and spatial memory performance in a rat model of stress. J Nutr Sci Vitaminol 61:1–7

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  66. Kang JS (2015) Exercise copes with prolonged stress-induced impairment of spatial memory performance by endoplasmic reticulum stress. J Exerc Nutr Biochem 19(3):191–197. https://doi.org/10.5717/jenb.2015.15080705

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Ozbeyli D, Gokalp AG, Koral T et al (2015) Protective effect of exercise and sildenafil on acute stress and cognitive function. Physiol Behav 151:230–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.07.030

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Radahmadi M, Alaei H, Sharifi MR et al (2015) Preventive and therapeutic effect of treadmill running on chronic stress-induced memory deficit in rats. J Bodyw Mov Ther 19(2):238–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbmt.2014.04.007

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Kim DM, Leem YH (2016) Chronic stress-induced memory deficits are reversed by regular exercise via AMPK-mediated BDNF induction. Neuroscience 324:271–285. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.03.019

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Wearick-Silva LE, Marshall P, Viola TW et al (2017) Running during adolescence rescues a maternal separation-induced memory impairment in female mice: potential role of differential exon-specific BDNF expression. Dev Psychobiol 59(2):268–274. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21487

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Chen K, Zhang L, Tan M et al (2017) Treadmill exercise suppressed stress-induced dendritic spine elimination in mouse barrel cortex and improved working memory via BDNF/TrkB pathway. Transl Psychiatry 7(3):e1069. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2017.41

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Dos Santos TM, Kolling J, Siebert C et al (2017) Effects of previous physical exercise to chronic stress on long-term aversive memory and oxidative stress in amygdala and hippocampus of rats. Int J Dev Neurosci 56:58–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2016.12.003

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Kochi C, Liu H, Zaidi S et al (2017) Prior treadmill exercise promotes resilience to vicarious trauma in rats. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 77:216–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.04.018

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Lapmanee S, Charoenphandhu J, Teerapornpuntakit J et al (2017) Agomelatine, venlafaxine, and running exercise effectively prevent anxiety- and depression-like behaviors and memory impairment in restraint stressed rats. PLoS One 12(11):e0187671. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187671

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Leem YH, Chang H (2017) Arc/Arg3.1 protein expression in dorsal hippocampal CA1, a candidate event as a biomarker for the effects of exercise on chronic stress-evoked behavioral abnormalities. J Exerc Nutr Biochem 21(4):45–51. https://doi.org/10.20463/jenb.2017.0033

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. da Silva DC, Tavares MG, do Nascimento CKB et al (2018) Can coconut oil and treadmill exercise during the critical period of brain development ameliorate stress-related effects on anxiety-like behavior and episodic-like memory in young rats? Food Funct 9(3):1492–1499. https://doi.org/10.1039/c7fo01516j

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Miller RM, Marriott D, Trotter J et al (2018) Running exercise mitigates the negative consequences of chronic stress on dorsal hippocampal long-term potentiation in male mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem 149:28–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2018.01.008

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul D. Loprinzi.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Author PL declares no conflict of interest. Author EF declares no conflict of interest.

Research involving human participants

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

No consent was needed as this is a review paper.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Loprinzi, P.D., Frith, E. Protective and therapeutic effects of exercise on stress-induced memory impairment. J Physiol Sci 69, 1–12 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12576-018-0638-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12576-018-0638-0

Keywords